"Unleash your creativity and unlock your potential with MsgBrains.Com - the innovative platform for nurturing your intellect." » English Books » Make It Stick: The Science of Successful Learning

Add to favorite Make It Stick: The Science of Successful Learning

Select the language in which you want the text you are reading to be translated, then select the words you don't know with the cursor to get the translation above the selected word!




Go to page:
Text Size:

Notes to Pages 123–131 ê 274

18. Stories on student- directed learning: Susan Dominus, “Play-Dough? Calculus? At the Manhattan Free School, Anything Goes,” New York Times, October 4, 2010, and Asha Anchan,

“The DIY Approach to Education,” Minneapolis StarTribune, July 8, 2012.

19. Studies showing that students drop fl ashcards sooner than they should for long- term learning include N. Kornell & R. A.

Bjork, Optimizing self-

regulated study: The benefi ts— and

costs— of dropping fl ashcards, Memory 16 (2008), 125– 136, and J. D. Karpicke, Metacognitive control and strategy selection: Deciding to practice retrieval during learning, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 138 (2009), 469– 486.

20. Eric Mazur has published Peer Instruction: A User’s Manual, about his approach to teaching. (Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice- Hall, 1997). In addition, he exemplifi es his approach in an engaging YouTube lecture, “Confessions of a converted lecturer,” described in Note 15. Again, it is http:// www .youtube

.com /watch ?v=WwslBPj8GgI, accessed October 23, 2013.

21. The Dunning quote comes from E. Morris, “The anosognosic’s dilemma: Something’s wrong but you’ll never know what it is”

(pt. 5), New York Times, June 24, 2010.

22. Peter Brown interview of Catherine Johnson, December 13, 2011, Minneapolis, MN.

23. Much of this chapter is about how to regulate one’s learning while avoiding various illusions and biases based on fl uency, hindsight bias, and the like. An excellent recent article on self-regulated learning that would prove useful to anyone seeking more knowledge on these topics is R. A. Bjork, J. Dunlosky, & N. Kornell, Self- regulated learning: Beliefs, techniques, and illusions, Annual Review of Psychology 64 (2013), 417– 444.

6. Get Beyond Learning Styles

1. Francis Bacon (1561– 1626) was an En glish phi los o pher and statesman. The full quote is “All rising a to great place is by a winding stair; and if there be factions, it is good to side a man’s self, whilst he is in the rising, and to balance himself when he is placed.” From Bacon’s essay Of Great Place.

Notes to Pages 133–148 ê 275

2. Peter Brown interview of Bruce Hendry, August 27, 2012, St.

Paul, MN. All quotes of Hendry are from this interview.

3. Betsy Morris, Lisa Munoz, and Patricia Neering, “Overcoming dyslexia,” Fortune, May, 2002, 54– 70.

4. Annie Murphy Paul, “The upside of dyslexia,” New York Times, February 4, 2012. The work by Geiger and Lettvin is described in G. Geiger & J. Y. Lettvin, Developmental dyslexia: A different perceptual strategy and how to learn a new strategy for reading, Saggi: Child Development and Disabilities 26

(2000), 73– 89.

5. Survey is listed in F. Coffi eld, D. Moseley, E. Hall, Learning styles and pedagogy in post- 16 learning, a systematic and critical review, 2004, Learning and Skills Research Centre, London; the quote by the student (“there’s no point in me reading a book”) is from same source, p. 137. The quote “a bedlam of contradictory claims” is from Michael Reynolds, Learning styles: a critique, Management Learning, June 1997, vol. 28 no. 2, p. 116.

6. The material about learning styles is drawn largely from H. Pashler, M. A. McDaniel, D. Rohrer, & R. A. Bjork, Learning styles: A critical review of concepts and evidence, Psychological Science in the Public Interest 9 (2009), 105– 119. This article reviewed the published evidence bearing on whether learning is improved when the instructional method is matched to students’ learning styles relative to when the instructional method is not matched. Two important fi ndings were that (1) there are very few studies that adopted the gold standard of performing controlled experiments, and (2) the few published experiments consistently found that matching instruction to learning style did not improve learning. One key conclusion is that more experimental research on this issue is needed, but at the moment there is little evidence for the existence of commonly postulated learning styles.

7. An excellent text on classic views of intelligence is Earl Hunt, Human intelligence (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2010).

8. Howard Gardner’s theory is described in his book Multiple Intelligences: New Horizons (New York: Basic Books, 2006), among other venues.

Notes to Pages 148–150 ê 276

9. The material on work by Robert Sternberg, Elena Grigorenko, and their colleagues comes from several sources. For a nice pre sen ta tion of the theory, see R. J. Sternberg, Grigorenko, E. L.,

& Zhang, L., Styles of learning and thinking in instruction and assessment, Perspectives on Psychological Science (2008) 486– 506. Another interesting study by Sternberg, Grigorenko and colleagues identifi ed college students who showed much higher skill in either analytical, creative, or practical ability (relative to the other two abilities), and assigned them to different classes that focused on analytic instruction, creative instruction, or practical instruction. Students receiving instruction that matched their strongest ability tended to perform better on certain class- performance assessments than students who received mismatched instruction; see R. J. Sternberg, E. L.

Grigorenko, M. Ferrari, & P. Clinkenbeard, A triarchic analysis of an aptitude– treatment interaction, Eu ro pe an Journal of Psychological Assessment 15 (1999), 1– 11.

10. The study of Brazilian children was T. N. Carraher, D. W. Carraher, & A. D. Schliemann, Mathematics in the streets and in the schools, British Journal of Developmental Psychology 3

(1985), 21– 29. This fascinating study focused on fi ve children from very poor backgrounds who were working on street corners or markets in Brazil. Per for mance was compared for similar multiplication problems presented in different contexts: the natural context in which the child was expert (e.g., selling coconuts, but role- played in the experiment), word problems phrased within a different context (e.g., selling bananas), or formal math problems without context. The children solved nearly 100 percent of the problems when presented in the natural context, fewer in the different context, and only about a third when presented as a formal problem. A key point is that the children used concrete grouping strategies to solve the natural context problems, but then switched to school- taught strategies (not yet well learned) when presented with the formal problems. The mathematical strategies the children had developed were not evident on an academically oriented test.

11. The study of race handicappers is S. J. Ceci & J. K. Liker, A day at the races: A study of IQ, expertise, and cognitive complex-

Notes to Pages 152–155 ê 277

ity, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 115 (1986), 255– 266. This study sampled harness racing fans, with some classifi ed as expert and some as less expert. The expert group and less expert group were evenly matched on IQ, yet the expert group showed much better success at predicting outcomes of actual races and experimenter- contrived races. The experts’

success was related to their using an extremely complex system of weighting and combining the range of information related to the horses and the race conditions.

12. Dynamic testing: Robert Sternberg and Elena Grigorenko discuss this concept in Dynamic Testing: The Nature and Mea-sure ment of Learning Potential (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2002).

13. The fundamental work on structure building was begun by M. A. Gernsbacher, K. R. Varner, & M. E. Faust, Investigating differences in general comprehension skills, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 16

(1990), 430– 445. This article provides some of the elegant experimental work that contributed to the development of the structure- building theory— the idea that good comprehenders are able to construct a coherent, or ga nized repre sen ta tion of a narrative from many sources (either read, listened to, or seen in pictures), whereas less able comprehenders tend to construct many, somewhat fractionated repre sen ta tions of the narratives.

This research further suggested that poor structure-builders, but not good structure-builders, have trouble inhibiting irrelevant information, which likely contributes to their fractionated (in-effec tive) repre sen ta tions. Another relevant article is A. A. Callender & M. A. McDaniel, The benefi ts of embedded question adjuncts for low and high structure builders, Journal of Educational Psychology 99 (2007), 339– 348. They demonstrated that low structure-builders achieve less learning from standard school materials (textbook chapters) than do high structure-builders. However, embedding questions into chapters to focus the low structure-builders on the important concepts (and requiring them to answer the questions) boosted the low structure-builders to levels of learning enjoyed by high structure- builders.

Notes to Pages 157–158 ê 278

Are sens

Copyright 2023-2059 MsgBrains.Com